Brain's
Clock Has Millisecond Hand
By Karen Heyman
ScienceNOW Daily News
1 February 2007
Were
it not for our brain's ability to sense time--even on the millisecond
scale--Morse code would sound like one long beep. Now researchers
are uncovering the first clues into how this mental clock works,
even when we're not sending a telegraph.
For decades, neuroscientists believed the brain had a specific region
dedicated to keeping track of time. Recently, this view has begun
to change. Instead of a single internal chronometer, many researchers
now believe that the natural firings of neurons throughout the brain
may give us our ability to sense the passage of time. Exactly how
this works, however, has remained a mystery.
To
probe the inner workings of our mental clock, University of California
(UC), Los Angeles, neuroscientist Dean Buonomano and Uma Karmarkar,
now a post-doctoral fellow at UC Berkeley, focused on an idea called
state-dependency. The concept goes like this: Imagine you are dripping
red paint into a can of white paint. The first drop falls into pure
white paint, but with each successive drop, the red paint enters
a pinker and pinker solution.
Buonomano
and Karmarkar hypothesized that something similar goes on in the
brain. When you hear the first "beep" of Morse code, for
example, a specific set of neurons fire. This causes the brain to
be in a different state than it was before it heard the sound. As
a result, when the next beep sounds, the brain clicks into another
state. Keeping track of the differences between these states allows
the brain to mark time, the team showed in a computer model that
replicates neuron interactions.
To
confirm the model, the scientists had human volunteers judge the
interval between two beeps. Some beeps were played close together,
others farther apart. Most subjects could easily tell the difference,
but when the researchers played a "distracter" tone before
the beeps, the volunteers had difficulty judging the time interval,
the team reports in today's issue of Neuron. That's because the
distracter tone confuses the brain's initial reference point, says
Buonomano. It's the difference between throwing pebbles into a still
pond or one that's already rippled, he notes. Only in the still
pond can you correctly perceive the pattern of your pebble's ripples.
"This
is a beautiful paper," says Warren Meck, a cognitive neuroscientist
at Duke University in Durham, North Carolina. "[It] has important
implications for our everyday perception of the temporal relationships
among all of the sights and sounds that we process," he says.
Related
site
Science article on how the brain processes time