Evolution
and the brain
Top of page
Abstract
With all deference to the sensibilities of religious people, the idea
that man was created in the image of God can surely be put aside.
The vast
majority of scientists, and the majority of religious people, see little
potential for pleasure or progress in the conflicts between religion
and science that are regularly fanned into flame by a relatively small
number on both sides of the debate. Many scientists are religious, and
perceive no conflict between the values of their science — values that
insist on disinterested, objective inquiry into the nature of the Universe
— and those of their faith.
But there
are lines that should not be crossed, and in a recent defence of his
beliefs and disbeliefs in the matter of evolution, US Senator Sam Brownback
(Republican, Kansas) crosses at least one. Senator Brownback was one
of three Republican presidential candidates who, in a recent debate,
described himself as not believing in evolution. He sought to explain
his position with greater nuance in a 31 May article in The New York
Times, in which he wrote: "Man was not an accident and reflects
an image and likeness unique in the created order. Those aspects of
evolutionary theory compatible with this truth are a welcome addition
to human knowledge. Aspects of these theories that undermine this truth,
however, should be firmly rejected as atheistic theology posing as science."
Humans
evolved, body and mind, from earlier primates. The ways in which humans
think reflect this heritage as surely as the ways in which their limbs
are articulated, their immune systems attack viruses and the cones in
their eyes process coloured light. This applies not just to the way
in which our neurons fire, but also to various aspects of our moral
thought, as we report this week in a News Feature on the moral connotations
of disgust (see page 768). The way that disgust functions in our lives
and shapes our moral decisions reflects not just cultural training,
but also biological evolution. Current theorizing on this topic, although
fascinating, may be wide of the mark. But its basis in the idea that
human minds are the product of evolution is not atheistic theology.
It is unassailable fact.
This does
not utterly invalidate the idea that the human mind is, as Senator Brownback
would have it, a reflection of the mind of God. But the suggestion that
any entity capable of creating the Universe has a mind encumbered with
the same emotional structures and perceptual framework as that of an
upright ape adapted to living in small, intensely social peer-groups
on the African savannah seems a priori unlikely.
In Brownback's
defence, it should be acknowledged that these are deep waters. It is
fairly easy to accept the truth of evolution when it applies to the
external world — the adaptation of the orchid to wasps, for example,
or the speed of the cheetah. It is much harder to accept it internally
— to accept that our feelings, intuitions, the ways in which we love
and loathe, are the product of experience, evolution and culture alone.
And such acceptance has challenges for the unbeliever, too. Moral philosophers
often put great store by their rejection of the 'naturalistic fallacy',
the belief that because something is a particular way, it ought to be
that way. Now we learn that untutored beliefs about 'what ought to be'
do, in fact, reflect an 'is': the state of the human mind as an evolved
entity. Accepting this represents a challenge that few as yet have really
grappled with.
It remains
uncertain how the new sciences of human behaviour emerging at the intersections
of anthropology, evolutionary biology and neuropsychology can best be
navigated. But that does not justify their denunciation on the basis
of religious faith alone. Scientific theories of human nature may be
discomforting or unsatisfying, but they are not illegitimate. And serious
attempts to frame them will reflect the origins of the human mind in
biological and cultural evolution, without reference to a divine creation.